Skip to main content
  • Systematic Review
  • Open access
  • Published:

A meta-analysis of behaviour change techniques in social interventions targeting improved cognitive function in older adults

Abstract

Background

Limited social networks in older adults are linked with increased dementia risk. However, there is a lack of knowledge on whether socially-based behavioural interventions (i.e., programs designed to increase individual’s social opportunities, engagement or networks) can improve cognitive function, as well as the role of applied behaviour change techniques (BCTs) in effective interventions. This systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to (i) quantify the effectiveness of social-based behavioural interventions in improving cognition in older adults, and (ii) identify which BCTs increase social activity behaviour of older adults.

Methods

Six electronic databases were searched with restrictions for age (>65 years) and English language from inception to July 2023 (PROSPERO:CRD42021283382) for articles reporting social-based behavioural randomised controlled trials and using a measured outcome of cognitive function. Behaviour change techniques were mapped to the BCT V1 model and risk of bias was assessed. Pooled effect sizes from eligible studies were synthesised using RevMan.

Results

We identified 9528 records and included 15 studies (N=1785 participants). Meta-analyses showed that social-based interventions had a medium effect on global cognition (d=0.80, 95% CI 0.58 to 1.02, p<0.01), but not executive function. The most frequently used intervention components were social-based communication (e.g., chatting, boosting social engagement), group arts-based tasks (e.g., knitting, music, craft) and guided reminiscence. The BCT demonstration of behaviour predicted significant cognitive effects and explained 94.6% of inter-study variation.

Discussion

Findings carry implications for developing comprehensive strategies to promote social initiatives supporting cognitive health, particularly in addressing the challenges faced by older adults.

Peer Review reports

Background

Increase in life expectancy and population ageing is a global phenomenon [1]. An increasingly high number of older adults, especially women, are living alone [2]. About half of those 60 years and older are at risk of being socially isolated and approximately 30% of the oldest old (aged 85+ years) may experience some degree of loneliness [3,4,5,6]. Social isolation, particularly among older adults living alone, has been linked to poorer eating habits, challenges in instrumental activities of daily living, and a heightened vulnerability to a range of health issues, including recent illness and falls [7,8,9]. Additionally, individuals experiencing social isolation face an elevated risk of various adverse health outcomes, such as depression, cardiovascular diseases, diabetes, cardiovascular death, and non-cardiovascular death [10].

Albeit less widely recognised, social isolation among older adults is associated with an increased risk of dementia and cognitive decline [11,12,13,14], attributed to factors such as diminished cognitive stimulation, heightened loneliness and depression, chronic stress, inflammation, and impaired neuroplasticity [15, 16]. Consequently, addressing social isolation and enabling social engagement may present important strategies for mitigating dementia risk and maintaining cognition in older age. We aimed to examine evidence that intervening to enhance social engagement could improve cognition in older adults.

Social isolation and loneliness are distinct but interrelated concepts. While loneliness is the subjective feeling of being lonely, social isolation is typically defined as the objective lack or limited extent of social contact (e.g., varying due to marital status, living alone or with others) [17, 18]. The prevalence of social isolation varies across different studies. Recent evidence showed higher rates in middle and high-income countries and urban areas [10]. However, previous studies have shown prevalence values ranging from 20% to 34% in European countries [19], 31% in Japan [20], 24% in the USA [21], 14% in Australia [22], and 15% in Brazil [23].

The significant prevalence of social isolation among older adults, coupled with its documented adverse effects on health and wellbeing, emphasises the imperative to address social isolation as a public health concern. Despite the robust evidence supporting interventions targeted at mitigating social isolation and the implementation of strategies to enhance its impact (e.g., [24,25,26,27,28,29,30]), a substantial gap persists in understanding the characteristics and effectiveness of interventions aimed at improving cognition among these individuals. Therefore, elucidating the mechanisms by which social isolation influences cognitive function and exploring the potential efficacy of interventions targeting both social isolation and cognitive decline are necessary avenues for future research in gerontology and public health. However, bridging the gap between evidence-backed interventions and their effective implementation is compounded by the challenge of recruiting socially isolated individuals which necessitates thoughtful approaches to ensure the success of intervention strategies.

Empirical studies have identified preliminary efficacy of social-based behavioural interventions in ameliorating social isolation and loneliness across individual, community and societal dimensions [31,32,33]. These interventions often encompass methodologies or programs crafted to alter or influence individual behaviours within social environments or contexts [34,35,36,37]. For instance, social-based behavioural initiatives strategically use social interactions, norms, and networks to stimulate behavioural change, advocate healthier decision-making, or tackle targeted issues such as chronic health outcomes, environmental sustainability, or community engagement [38,39,40]. They frequently rely on mechanisms such as social support, peer influence, social norms, or community resources to facilitate constructive behavioural transformations and augment overall wellbeing [40, 41]. Individual-focused interventions, encompassing both face-to-face and digital modalities, include social skills training, peer support, social activity groups, befriending services, and cognitive-behavioural therapy [26, 42, 43]. Community-level interventions target enhancements in transportation, improvements to the built environment's accessibility, and digital inclusion [44, 45]. At the societal level, interventions concentrate on augmenting social cohesion and mitigating marginalisation [31].

Central to these interventions are active components strategically designed to induce behavioural change, which are often encapsulated within Michie et al.'s [46] hierarchical international taxonomy of 93 Behaviour Change Techniques (BCTs), facilitating consensus in reporting behavioural change interventions and contributing to enhanced clarity and standardisation in intervention research. Building on the established role of BCTs in addressing risk factors associated with chronic health conditions such as diabetes [47,48,49], BCT taxonomies have also been specifically tailored for modifying unhealthy habits, such as smoking [50] and alcohol consumption [51]. Despite the wealth of information on BCT applications in varied health contexts, a gap exists regarding the identification of BCTs employed to encourage older adults to enhance social engagement and the potential cognitive benefits that could be derived from such interventions.

Therefore, this review aimed to identify the effectiveness of social-based behavioural interventions on cognition in older individuals while offering a comprehensive synthesis of the BCTs embedded within these social programs.

Methods

Registration

We used the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Protocol (PRISMA-P) [52] to guide reporting (see Additional file 1, Supplementary Table 1). This protocol was registered on the PROSPERO database (CRD42021283382) before the search commenced.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Randomised controlled trials were eligible for inclusion if they involved reported use of a social program (intervention) compared to usual care, active/passive control or no intervention (comparator), and included at least one measure of cognition (outcomes). In cases where the age range was not explicitly stated, we took an inclusive approach and included studies where the mean age of participants was over 65 years. While there may have been studies with mixed age groups or only reported mean or median age, articles were included if the focus was on older adults to maintain consistency and relevance to our research question.

Due to the broad definition of “social” interventions, articles were included if the main component contained any type of social-based interventions focused on alleviating loneliness and/or targeted improvement in social behaviour. This may include but was not restricted to structured reminiscence individual or group therapy, social group gatherings/excursions, psychodynamic therapy, mindfulness therapy, cognitive-based interventions, videoconference program, peer support network, laughter therapy, broad public health campaigns (e.g., media campaigns), web and smartphone applications. That is, interventions were included that contained some element of contact and participation with other people (beyond the research team). Interventions were also considered social-based behavioural programs if they explicitly articulated social aims or incorporated components designed to modify social interactions, norms, or networks. This might therefore encompass interventions building social engagement, promoting peer support, facilitating social interactions, or targeting social norms.

Studies were also only included if cognitive performance was one of the measured outcomes. Studies were excluded if it included populations with a previous history or symptoms of dementia (e.g., Alzheimer's disease), or mild cognitive impairment. There were no restrictions on gender type, occupation, or living arrangements.

Search strategy

A systematic search of six electronic databases MEDLINE, EMBASE, PsycINFO, PubMed, Ovid and CINAHL was conducted with the assistance of a trained librarian from inception to 9 August 2022, and an updated search was conducted on 7 July 2023 to capture any new publications from the original search timepoint for review finalisation. Search terms were based on a combination of descriptors including MeSH terms for social isolation (social alienation OR social isolation OR social distance OR isolation OR loneliness OR social connectivity OR social environment), older adults (aged OR older adult* OR older person*OR aged, 80 and over OR geriatric OR older senior) with an intervention focus (intervention* OR program* OR education* OR treatment* OR behavio* therapy OR health promotion*). Search criteria targeted peer-reviewed articles in English, restricted to older adults and to randomised controlled trials. The complete search strategy for one database is shown in Additional file 1, Supplementary Table 1. In addition, reference lists of eligible papers were screened for relevant articles.

Study selection

Potential studies were exported into Rayyan with duplicates deleted by the primary author (JS). The initial screening of article titles and abstracts was conducted for the first 500 articles by three researchers (JS, VC, MA) to verify eligibility and calculate inter-rater reliability (95%). All discrepancies were discussed with the larger research team and resolved by consensus. The remainder of the papers were screened at title and abstract level by two researchers (VC, MA) independently. Inclusion of full text articles was completed independently by two research members (VC, MA) and checked by the primary author (JS). All steps of paper identification and selection are presented in the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta- Analysis (PRISMA) diagram (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA diagram

Data extraction

Data from eligible studies were extracted independently by three reviewers (VC, MA, MJRP) and verified by the primary author (JS). We used a pre-designed data extraction sheet to allow standardised reporting of results across studies, including information on: (1) study characteristics (e.g., study design, year, country, population group), (2) participant characteristics, (3) assessments used, (4) outcomes, and (5) study findings.

BCTs coding and assumptions

Coding of the behaviour change techniques identified in the eligible studies was completed using established BCT taxonomies [46] and strategies described in previous research [53]. BCTs were coded where there was clear evidence of their application in the interventions described. The coders (VC and MJRP) received training from the research team members who are experienced in using BCT taxonomies and in the development of interventions for health promotion (FM) and implementation science (JS). Coders rated BCTs as either present or absent in the intervention arms separately with two rounds of separate coding required to achieve a suitable inter-rater agreement (first round, 84%, second round 95%). Discrepancies were brought to the research team, resolved through discussion until consensus was met, and updated accordingly. During the coding process, all interventions were scored for the presence of BCTs using the definition that BCTs aimed to modify participants' behaviour. For instance, a program aimed at enhancing social interaction among older adults may include established BCTs such as providing social support, setting social goals, and facilitating group discussions. These would be scored as present within this intervention because they are intended to influence individuals' behaviours towards engaging in more social interactions, the target of the intervention. Furthermore, in instances where interventions were described as provision of education or information without further detail, it was presumed that any informational session within an implementation intervention would encompass, at the very least, two BCTs: offering information on consequences and instruction on behaviour execution. However, additional BCTs were identified if observed. Finally, when interventions were described as providing "training" without further detail, it was inferred that, unless explicitly stated otherwise, any intervention featuring a training session would incorporate instruction on behaviour execution. A list of BCTs and their definition, as well as resulting codes from each study is provided in Additional file 1, Supplementary Table 2.

Risk of bias

Study rigour was measured through risk of bias ratings of included studies which was conducted by three members (JS, VC, MJRP) of the research team using the Cochrane tool for assessing Risk of Bias in randomised trials (RoB2 tool, [54]) and confirmed by the primary author (JS) for further validation. The risk of bias tool covers the following domains of bias: (a) selection bias which includes sequence generation and allocation concealment, (b) performance bias which detects the blinding of participants and personnel, (c) blinding of outcome assessment, (d) attrition bias, and (e) reporting bias to determine level of bias (using a traffic light system of high, low or some concern). At the end, the overall risk of bias was set as low if the study was judged to be at low risk of bias for all domains [54]. A study was rated as some concerns if the study was judged to raise some concerns in at least one domain for this result, but not to be at high risk of bias for any domain. Studies with high risk of bias were those judged to be at high risk of bias in at least one domain, or the study was judged to have some concerns for multiple domains in a way that substantially lowered confidence in the results.

Data analysis

Studies were grouped by intervention type and cognitive outcomes. Outcome measures that were assessed in at least two eligible RCTs using the same intervention (cf. control condition) were included in a separate meta-analysis [55, 56]. Reported outcome measures that were continuous in nature were translated to a standardised effect size (Hedges’ g = (mi-mc)/sdic). RevMan (v5.3) was used to produce d and SEd, and forest plots, and estimates of the pooled effect and heterogeneity index I2 across the four outcomes (whereby 50–90% was considered as representing substantial heterogeneity). RevMan uses study sample size to weight effect sizes in a meta-analysis. Given the use of various outcome measures and intervention designs, it was improbable for our included studies to have shared an identical true effect size and thus a random-effects model was adopted. We reported on data closest to the intervention’s endpoint. Statistical significance of meta-analysis was set at p<0.05.

To investigate heterogeneity in main effects analyses, moderator effects of BCTs were explored using restricted maximum likelihood random effects meta-regressions. Univariate meta-regressions were carried out to examine the association between individual BCTs that were present (vs. not) and the effect of the number of BCTs used. Meta-regressions were only performed when there was evidence of substantial heterogeneity (I2 ≥ 50%), ≥ 10 trials per analysis [57], and at least four trials using a BCT, to minimise the impact of single trials.

Results

Overview

Database searches returned 9528 eligible studies for title and abstract screening, with fifteen articles ultimately deemed eligible from full-text screening and included in our final synthesis which is described from hereon [58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72] (Fig. 1).

Fifteen trials with 1,785 participants (range 20–348) were included in this review. Participants across the trials had a mean age of 76 years (range= 68.0–83.5 years), and the majority were women (70.6%, n=1261). Studies were conducted in the United States (n=2), Japan (n=4), China (n=1), Finland (n=1), Denmark (n=1), Ireland (n=1), Australia (n=1), Brazil (n=1), Spain (n=2) and Germany (n=1) (Table 1). The interventions took place mostly in the community (n=9, 60%) [59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72]. Table 1 further describes summary characteristics of included trial populations and detailed information about each trial included in this analysis, including all reported outcomes.

Table 1 Summary of included studies (n=15)

Interventions

Table 2 describes the summary characteristics of each intervention group. Social-based behavioural programs all aimed at building social connections, cognitive stimulation, and emotional well-being in older adults. The most common program types included reminiscence activities (e.g., group reminiscence approach, reality orientation; n=4, 26.7%) [59, 61, 63, 65], arts-based group endeavours (e.g., group choir music, quilt making, activities and discussions around art and therapeutic writing, photography; n=4, 26.7%) [58, 68, 72] and social-based communication (e.g. video chats, assignment of a communication robot; n=4, 26.7%) [60, 64, 67, 69], followed by cognitive-based group tasks (e.g. board games and puzzles; n=2, 13.3%) [62, 65] and education or social awareness building programs (n=2, 13.3%) [70, 71]. All except for one study [60] delivered group interventions face-to-face.

Table 2 Intervention characteristics

On average, the interventions lasted 28 weeks (range 6–48 weeks), with an average retention rate of 84% (range 68–100). Apart from three studies [61, 63, 69], all studies had interventions that were delivered at least once every week.

Outcomes

The majority of the studies measured global cognitive function using self-reported tools (n = 14; with the clinician-administered Mini-Mental Status Examination most commonly reported (MMSE, n = 11)). Several of the studies also assessed executive function (n = 7), which often used the Trail Making Test (TMT-A (n = 3); TMT-B (n=2)). Two studies measured quality of life using self-reported measures (e.g., Quality of life Face Scale Score).

Figures 2 and 3 present the results of the random-effects meta-analysis estimating the mean change from pre-intervention to post-intervention for global cognition and executive function. Of the studies included, 10 provided sufficient data for inclusion in the meta-analysis for global cognition, and 6 for executive function. Overall, social-based behavioural interventions significantly improved global cognition (d = 0.80, 95% CI: [0.58, 1.02], k = 10, p < 0.001) with moderate heterogeneity (I2=45%) (Fig. 2). Social-based behavioural interventions had no significant effects on executive function (d = 0.62, 95% CI: [−0.88, 2.11], k = 6, p = 0.42) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot of social interventions and effects on global cognition

Fig. 3
figure 3

Forest plot of social interventions and effects on executive function

In the three studies that scored acceptable in five or more risk of bias dimensions [62, 69, 71], two reported beneficial changes in global cognition [62, 71].

Behaviour change techniques

Table 3 shows the BCTs coded for each of the 15 studies. Out of 93 possible BCTs, we identified 20 BCTs (21.5%) targeting changes in the social behaviour of participants. Out of 16 possible BCT categories, 12 categories (75%) were coded, with ‘Associations’, ‘Reward and threat’, ‘Scheduled consequences’, and ‘Covert learning’ not included. The most commonly applied BCTs targeting behaviours included: 1) goal setting (15/15, 100%); 2) action planning (15/15, 100%); 3) instruction on how to perform a behaviour (15/15, 100%); 4) demonstration of the behaviour (8/15, 53.3%); 5) monitoring of emotional consequences (6/15, 40%); 6) credible source (6/15, 40%), and 7) adding objects to the environment (6/15, 40%). The average number of BCTs used in each study were 6 BCTs (range 3–15).

Table 3 Behaviour change techniques identified in the included studies

A number of BCTs were identified in the five studies that showed benefits on global cognition. Major differences found between identified BCTs in effective compared to non-effective interventions indicated that most popular BCTs of feedback and self-monitoring of behaviour were associated with positive intervention effect (Table 3). Goal setting of behaviour, action planning, demonstration of behaviour and having credible sources were also identified, however these were also present in non-effective interventions. Demonstration of outcome (BCT6) was significantly associated with outcome effect (b = −0.602; 95% CI: −1.195 to −0.009). This model predicted 94.6% of the variance (Table 4).

Table 4 Results from meta-regression analysis of social-based behavioural interventions

Methodological quality

The overall risk of bias was high for 14/15 studies (93.3%) (Fig. 4). The risk of bias was judged low for selective reporting in 11 of the studies (73.3%). Four studies (26.7%) were judged to be at a high risk of bias related to the lack of random sequence generation, 6 (40%) were judged to be at a high risk due to lack of concealed allocation and blinding of participants and personnel, 4 (26.7%) were judged to be at a high risk of bias related to lack of blinding of outcome assessment, 2 (13.3%) were judged to be at a high risk due to having incomplete outcome data, and 4 (26.7%) were assessed as susceptible to other potential sources of bias. Out of the 15 studies, only 1 (6.7%) had consistent low risk of bias in all six categories.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Risk of bias summary

Discussion

This systematic review and meta-analysis provided a comprehensive examination of behaviour change techniques applied in social interventions resulting in improved cognition in older adults. Three BCTs were identified to be present in all 15 studies (goal setting of the behaviour, action planning and instruction on how to perform a behaviour), however only the BCT of comparison of behaviour was associated with improved cognitive outcomes. Our analysis highlights the potential of interventions that increase social engagement to improve cognitive health among older adults, and emphasises a role for behaviour change techniques in achieving positive cognitive outcomes.

Effectiveness of interventions: global cognition

Our findings suggest that social intervention programs can positively impact on global cognition, but not specifically on executive function. Multiple studies that investigated the impact of social activity on global cognition suggest that social intervention programs may be useful for promoting brain health in older adults. This is likely due to a boost in neuroplasticity affecting large-scale brain network connectivity and function, conferring generalised global cognitive benefits [73]. Larger social networks and greater levels of social support have also been associated with positive effects on global cognition [74]. However, there is a dual effect where higher levels of engagement promote positive cognitive outcome and better cognitive functioning is related to living a more engaged lifestyle [75, 76]. Having said this, in order to conduct a meta-analysis as we have done here, it was necessary to group interventions by common outcomes, rather than by the characteristics of the interventions themselves, particularly given the small number of studies identified that addressed our research question. Future work could seek to delineate the unique contributions of social interventions on a range of cognitive domains.

Social interactions are hypothesized to increase cognitive reserve through two pathways: bridging (provide cognitive stimulation via doing activities with others) and bonding (reduced stress via close relationships) [70]. It may be that certain types of social interactions have an influence on specific cognitive domains. The lack of association between social interventions and changes in specific cognitive domains may be related to differences in types of studies and the outcomes they included. For instance, only five studies assessed executive function, with most comprising of informal conversation groups. The only study to show a change in executive function was an arts based program which may have promoted planning skills [72]. Future research is needed to identify whether specific cognitive domains are influenced by specific kinds of social interventions.

Whilst social activities show an improvement in global cognition and increased brain volume, studies have highlighted that social intervention programs do not seem to affect specific cognitive domains such as memory, attention, or executive function [74]. A potential reason behind the contrasting outcomes observed between the influences of social support and engagement in social activities or networks could stem from the distinct role that social support plays in managing stress. Research indicates that social support contributes to building resilience against the detrimental effects of stress, which may have a buffering effect, helping to preserve cognition in older age [74]. In contrast, merely participating in social activities or having a broader circle of family and friends might not encompass the necessary social and emotional backing that is instrumental in reaping stress-alleviating advantages or the advantages of cognitive stimulation provided by doing activities which promote the use of specific cognitive skills.

BCTs and social behaviour

The BCT “demonstration of behaviour” is considered a key element within behaviour change interventions and is often observed in various systematic reviews spanning different fields [77,78,79,80]. This BCT involves the provision of real-life examples or models of the desired behaviour, which serves to illustrate the achievable outcomes and establish a reference point for individuals. Usually, demonstrating the behaviour in a tangible way offers a clear visual representation that can stimulate motivation and provide a sense of attainability. Previous systematic reviews have consistently highlighted the presence and impact of this BCT. In health interventions, such as exercise promotion or dietary changes [81,82,83,84], presenting role models or showcasing individuals who have successfully adopted the desired behaviour has been found effective in motivating others to follow suit. The human tendency to learn from and emulate others’ actions amplifies the potential of this technique. However, the effectiveness of behaviour demonstration hinges on factors such as relatability and authenticity of the models, as well as individual perceptions [85, 86]. Overly idealised representations might lead to feelings of inadequacy or disbelief and can negate the intended impact [87, 88]. Future social-based programs should consider how behaviour demonstration could be carried out, and provide tangible, relatable examples of desired actions to bridge the gap between intention and execution. By providing real-world instances of successful behaviour adoption, interventions can tap into social influence dynamics and inspire individuals to be more socially engaged.

The effectiveness of goal setting has been established in various domains, and its presence and impact have also been documented in multiple systematic reviews across multiple contexts [81, 82, 89, 90]. However, we were unable to identify its role in our current review. The efficacy of goal setting is likely contingent on factors like goal specificity, realism, and individual characteristics [91, 92] and these varied in the included studies. Research indicates that overly ambitious goals can lead to frustration and non-compliance, while overly simple goals might not challenge individuals adequately [93]. Moreover, goal setting might not be equally effective across all social conditions; its impact could vary depending on factors such as the complexity of the targeted behaviour and the individual’s level of commitment [94,95,96]. As such, its integrative role in future programs suggests its versatility and potential applicability in combating social issues such as isolation or engagement. As social interventions strive to bolster connectivity and improve mental well-being, goal setting could provide a structured approach for individuals to establish and pursue social participation objectives. Nevertheless, identifying BCTs can support the assessment of interventions that target several outcomes [97]. Whilst the quantity of BCTs is not necessarily associated with better outcomes, combinations of BCTs might increase its effectiveness [97].

Implications

Our review highlights a need for future interventions to prioritise robust BCT components and to acknowledge the potential impact of social interactions for promoting cognitive health among older adults. While the current meta-analysis offers new insights, the average duration of interventions remains short in nature (mean 23 months) and necessitates larger, longer-term trials. These lengthier trials would permit a more comprehensive understanding of the sustained effects of social-based interventions and facilitate additional explorations of the variability in outcomes and effectiveness of BCTs over extended periods. Such trials also need to be more robust in design to ensure compliance with randomisation sequences, blinding and full, not selective, reporting. Furthermore, given the observed variability in outcomes across demographic (e.g., gender and language) compositions, there is an additional implication for tailoring interventions based on demographic and cultural characteristics. Finally, we note that many studies did not include specific pre and post measures relating to social outcomes (e.g. social network size), and future research could examine whether the extent to which there are improvements in cognition is associated with measurable improvements on social measures. Policy and practice changes may need to incorporate specific considerations in the design and implementation of social interventions in order to enhance the efficacy of interventions and contribute to more equitable mental health outcomes among older adults.

Limitations

This review had a limited number of studies suitable for meta-analysis and synthesis. By only including studies published in English, there is a possibility we limited the number of available interventions for this review (e.g., latest study was in 2018). Furthermore, the inclusion of studies where cognition served as a secondary outcome rather than the primary focus is an additional limitation. In these instances, the interventions may not have been adequately powered to detect significant effects on cognition, as their design, sample size and power calculations may have primarily targeted other outcomes. This, in combination with multiple BCTs used and a variety of outcome measures, makes it difficult to allocate an effect size to a specific BCT. Further, because social interventions contain multiple modules with interactive components, it is difficult to attribute an improvement in a particular study outcome to one specific BCT. Additionally, it is important to recognise the limitations inherent in the use of specific instruments (e.g., TMT-A) as a proxy for executive function. Whilst we included several tests to measure a composite outcome for executive function, this was done to broaden the scope of assessment and increase the variability in the measures, thereby strengthening the overall analytical robustness. This decision aligns with the methodological imperative of enhancing the precision of effect size estimates and ensuring the reliability of the findings. As such, because of the emerging nature of this research field, which is very much in its infancy, the meta-analyses and meta-regressions were substantially underpowered, and we found that the majority of included studies had a risk of bias. This should also be taken into consideration with the fact that many studies contained small sample sizes and are likely at risk of type 1 error. Future work should seek to replicate these findings in larger trials and implementation studies.

Conclusion

There are a limited number of social-behavioural trials investigating the effectiveness of BCTs for the improvement of cognitive and mental health. The evidence to date supports the use of social interventions for improving global cognition in older adults, with comparison of behaviour being associated with positive changes. There is insufficient evidence linking social interventions to changes in specific cognitive domains, perhaps related to the discrepancy across interventions in the outcome measures they included. Moving forward, the findings advocate for the incorporation of social strategies into future interventions, emphasising their potential to yield clinically meaningful benefits for global cognition. Given these results, future research to identify the longer-term effects and sustainability of these approaches is warranted, which can contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of the enduring impact of social-based interventions on cognitive outcomes in older adults.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

  1. Lang JJ, Alam S, Cahill LE, Drucker AM, Gotay C, Kayibanda JF, et al. Global Burden of Disease Study trends for Canada from 1990 to 2016. Can Med Assoc J. 2018;190(44):E1296–304. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1503/cmaj.180698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Reher D, Requena M. Living Alone in Later Life: A Global Perspective. Popul Dev Rev. 2018;44(3):427–54. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/padr.12149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Ibrahim R, Abolfathi Momtaz Y, Hamid TA. Social isolation in older Malaysians: prevalence and risk factors. Psychogeriatrics. 2013;3(1479–8301):71–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/psyg.12000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Victor CR, Scambler SJ, Bowling ANN, Bond J. The prevalence of, and risk factors for, loneliness in later life: a survey of older people in Great Britain. Ageing Soc. 2005;25(6):357–75. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0144686X04003332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Grenade L, Boldy D. Social isolation and loneliness among older people: issues and future challenges in community and residential settings. Aust Health Rev. 2008;32(3):468–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1071/ah080468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Key W, Culliney M. The Oldest Old and the Risk of Social Exclusion. Soc Policy Soc. 2018;17(1):47–63. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/s1474746416000518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Negrini ELD, Nascimento CFD, Silva AD, Antunes JLF. Elderly persons who live alone in Brazil and their lifestyle. Revista Brasileira de Geriatria e Gerontologia. 2018;21(5):523–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1590/1981-22562018021.180101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Guo L, An L, Luo F, Yu B. Social isolation, loneliness and functional disability in Chinese older women and men: a longitudinal study. Age Ageing. 2021;50(4):1222–8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/ageing/afaa271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Ng N, Santosa A, Weinehall L, Malmberg G. Living alone and mortality among older people in Västerbotten County in Sweden: a survey and register-based longitudinal study. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20(1):7. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12877-019-1330-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Naito R, Leong DP, Bangdiwala SI, McKee M, Subramanian SV, Rangarajan S, et al. Impact of social isolation on mortality and morbidity in 20 high-income, middle-income and low-income countries in five continents. BMJ Glob Health. 2021;6(3):e004124. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1136/bmjgh-2020-004124.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015;10(2):227–37. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/1745691614568352.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Kuiper JS, Zuidersma M, Oude Voshaar RC, Zuidema SU, van den Heuvel ER, Stolk RP, et al. Social relationships and risk of dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2015;22:39–57. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.arr.2015.04.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Mahalingam G, Samtani S, Lam BCP, Lipnicki DM, Lima-Costa MF, Blay SL, et al. Social connections and risk of incident mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and mortality in 13 longitudinal cohort studies of ageing. Alzheimers Dement. 2023;19(11):5114–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/alz.13072.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Samtani S, Mahalingam G, Lam BCP, Lipnicki DM, Lima-Costa MF, Blay SL, et al. Associations between social connections and cognition: a global collaborative individual participant data meta-analysis. The Lancet Healthy Longevity. 2022;3(11):e740–53. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/s2666-7568(22)00199-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Seeman TE. Social ties and health: the benefits of social integration. Ann Epidemiol. 1996;6(5):442–51. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/s1047-2797(96)00095-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC. Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(10):447–54. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Valtorta N, Hanratty B. Loneliness, isolation and the health of older adults: do we need a new research agenda? J R Soc Med. 2012;105(12):518–22. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1258/jrsm.2012.120128.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Siette J, Pomare C, Dodds L, Jorgensen M, Harrigan N, Georgiou A. A comprehensive overview of social network measures for older adults: A systematic review. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2021;97:104525. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.archger.2021.104525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Yang K, Victor C. Age and loneliness in 25 European nations. Ageing Soc. 2011;31(8):1368–88. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1017/S0144686X1000139X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Shimada K, Yamazaki S, Nakano K, Ngoma AM, Takahashi R, Yasumura S. Prevalence of Social Isolation in Community-Dwelling Elderly by Differences in Household Composition and Related Factors: From a Social Network Perspective in Urban Japan. J Aging Health. 2014;26(5):807–23. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0898264314531616.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Cudjoe TKM, Roth DL, Szanton SL, Wolff JL, Boyd CM, Thorpe RJ. The Epidemiology of Social Isolation: National Health and Aging Trends Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2020;75(1):107–13. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/geronb/gby037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Beer A, Faulkner D, Law J, Lewin G, Tinker A, Buys L, et al. Regional variation in social isolation amongst older Australians. Reg Stud Reg Sci. 2016;3(1):170–84. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/21681376.2016.1144481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Duim E, Nascimento C, Silva A, Antunes JLF. Elderly persons who live alone in Brazil and their lifestyle. Revista Brasileira de Geriatria e Gerontologia. 2018;21(5):523–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1590/1981-22562018021.180101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Cattan M, White M, DBond J, Learmouth A. Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: a systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing and Society. 2005;25:41–67.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Dickens AP, Richards SH, Greaves CJ, Campbell JL. Interventions targeting social isolation in older people: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):647. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/1471-2458-11-647.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Fakoya OA, McCorry NK, Donnelly M. Loneliness and social isolation interventions for older adults: a scoping review of reviews. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):129. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12889-020-8251-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Franck L, Molyneux N, Parkinson L. Systematic review of interventions addressing social isolation and depression in aged care clients. Qual Life Res. 2016;25(6):1395–407. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s11136-015-1197-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hunter RF, De La Haye K, Murray JM, Badham J, Valente TW, Clarke M, et al. Social network interventions for health behaviours and outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 2019;16(9):e1002890. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002890.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Poscia A, Stojanovic J, La Milia DI, Duplaga M, Grysztar M, Moscato U, et al. Interventions targeting loneliness and social isolation among the older people: An update systematic review. Exp Gerontol. 2018;102:133–44. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.exger.2017.11.017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Shvedko A, Whittaker AC, Thompson JL, Greig CA. Physical activity interventions for treatment of social isolation, loneliness or low social support in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2018;34:128–37. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Cotterell N, Buffel T, Phillipon C. Preventing social isolation in older people. Maturitas. 2018;113:80–4. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.maturitas.2018.04.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Siette J, Jorgensen M, Nguyen A, Knaggs G, Miller S, Westbrook JI. A mixed-methods study evaluating the impact of an excursion-based social group on quality of life of older adults. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21(1):356. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12877-021-02295-7. Published 2021 Jun 10.

  33. Siette J, Ludlow K, Dodds L, Strutt P, Wuthrich V. "Provided a window on the world and lessened my feeling of isolation": older adults' perceived COVID-19 impact and technology use in Australia during recurrent lockdowns. BMC Geriatr. 2024;24(1):206. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12877-024-04807-7. Published 2024 Feb 28.

  34. Zhong X. Psychosocial Behavioral Intervention. Springer International Publishing; 2021. p. 4058-63.

  35. Darling SJ, Goods M, Ryan NP, Chisholm AK, Haebich K, Payne JM. Behavioral Intervention for Social Challenges in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2021;175(12):e213982. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.3982.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. National Research Council (US) Panel on Race Ethnicity and Health in Later Life. Behavioral Health Interventions: What Works and Why? . 2004. In: Critical Perspectives on Racial and Ethnic Differences in Health in Late Lifex. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US). Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK25527/?report=classic.

  37. Cella M, Lyne J, O’Donoghue B, Pillny M, Preti A, Riehle M, et al. Psychosocial and behavioural interventions for the negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a systematic review of efficacy meta-analyses. Br J Psychiatry. 2023;223(1):321–31. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1192/bjp.2023.21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Rau H, Nicolai S, Stoll-Kleemann S. A systematic review to assess the evidence-based effectiveness, content, and success factors of behavior change interventions for enhancing pro-environmental behavior in individuals. Front Psychol. 2022;13:901927. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.901927.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Kumar S, Preetha G. Health promotion: an effective tool for global health. Indian J Community Med. 2012;37(1):5–12. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.4103/0970-0218.94009.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Davis R, Campbell R, Hildon Z, Hobbs L, Michie S. Theories of behaviour and behaviour change across the social and behavioural sciences: a scoping review. Health Psychol Rev. 2015;9(3):323–44. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/17437199.2014.941722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Yamin P, Fei M, Lahlou S, Levy S. Using social norms to change behavior and increase sustainability in the real world: a systematic review of the literature. Sustainability. 2019;11(20):5847. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/su11205847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Webber M, Fendt-Newlin M. A review of social participation interventions for people with mental health problems. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52(4):369–80. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s00127-017-1372-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Siette J, Cassidy M, Priebe S. Effectiveness of befriending interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017;7(4):e014304. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014304. Published 2017 Apr 26.

  44. Ory MG, Lee C, Yoshikawa A. Health and the Built Environment. Springer International Publishing; 2021. p. 369-94.

  45. Sánchez-González D, Rojo-Pérez F, Rodríguez-Rodríguez V, Fernández-Mayoralas G. Environmental and psychosocial interventions in age-friendly communities and active ageing: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(22):8305. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/ijerph17228305.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Michie S, Richardson M, Johnston M, Abraham C, Francis J, Hardeman W, et al. The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Ann Behav Med. 2013;46(1):81–95. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Cradock KA, ÓLaighin G, Finucane FM, Gainforth HL, Quinlan LR, Ginis KAM. Behaviour change techniques targeting both diet and physical activity in type 2 diabetes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Physical Activ. 2017;14(1):18. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12966-016-0436-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Presseau J, Ivers NM, Newham JJ, Knittle K, Danko KJ, Grimshaw JM. Using a behaviour change techniques taxonomy to identify active ingredients within trials of implementation interventions for diabetes care. Implement Sci. 2015;10(1):55. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13012-015-0248-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Van Rhoon L, Byrne M, Morrissey E, Murphy J, McSharry J. A systematic review of the behaviour change techniques and digital features in technology-driven type 2 diabetes prevention interventions. DIGITAL HEALTH. 2020;6:2055207620914427. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/2055207620914427.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Michie S, Hyder N, Walia A, West R. Development of a taxonomy of behaviour change techniques used in individual behavioural support for smoking cessation. Addict Behav. 2011;36(4):315–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.11.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Michie S, Whittington C, Hamoudi Z, Zarnani F, Tober G, West R. Identification of behaviour change techniques to reduce excessive alcohol consumption. Addiction. 2012;107(8):1431–40. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03845.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n71. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1136/bmj.n71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Presseau J, Ivers NM, Newham JJ, Knittle K, Danko KJ, Grimshaw JM. Using a behaviour change techniques taxonomy to identify active ingredients within trials of implementation interventions for diabetes care. Implement Sci. 2015;10(1):55. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13012-015-0248-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Higgins J, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page M, et al. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.4: Cochrane; 2023.

  55. Deeks JJ, PT HJ, Altman De. Chapter 10: Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses. In: Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Welch VA. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 64: Cochrane; 2023.

  56. Friede T, Röver C, Wandel S, Neuenschwander B. Meta-analysis of two studies in the presence of heterogeneity with applications in rare diseases. Biom J. 2017;59(4):658–71. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/bimj.201500236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, Savovic J, Schulz KF, Weeks L, Sterne JA, Cochrane Bias Methods Group, & Cochrane Statistical Methods Group. The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ (Clinical research ed.). 2011;343:d5928. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1136/bmj.d5928.

  58. Ahessy B. The use of a music therapy choir to reduce depression and improve quality of life in older adults – a randomized control trial. Music Med. 2016;8:17–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.47513/mmd.v8i1.451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Akanuma K, Meguro K, Meguro M, Sasaki E, Chiba K, Ishii H, et al. Improved social interaction and increased anterior cingulate metabolism after group reminiscence with reality orientation approach for vascular dementia. Psychiatry Res. 2011;192(3):183–7. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.11.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Dodge HH, Zhu J, Mattek N, Bowman M, Ybarra O, Wild K, et al. Web-enabled Conversational Interactions as a Means to Improve Cognitive Functions: Results of a 6-Week Randomized Controlled Trial. Alzheimer’s Dementia. 2015;1(1):1–12. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.trci.2015.01.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Gudex C, Horsted C, Jensen AM, Kjer M, Sørensen J. Consequences from use of reminiscence - a randomised intervention study in ten Danish nursing homes. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10(1):33. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/1471-2318-10-33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, Kobayashi-Cuya KE, Kobayashi M, Inagaki H, et al. Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34(2):324–32. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/gps.5024.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Meléndez-Moral JC, Charco-Ruiz L, Mayordomo-Rodríguez T, Sales-Galán A. Effects of a reminiscence program among institutionalized elderly adults. Psicothema. 2013;25(3):319–23. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.7334/psicothema2012.253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Mortimer JA, Ding D, Borenstein AR, DeCarli C, Guo Q, Wu Y, et al. Changes in brain volume and cognition in a randomized trial of exercise and social interaction in a community-based sample of non-demented Chinese elders. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;30(4):757–66. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3233/jad-2012-120079.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Nakatsuka M, Nakamura K, Hamanosono R, Takahashi Y, Kasai M, Sato Y, et al. A cluster randomized controlled trial of nonpharmacological interventions for old-old subjects with a clinical dementia rating of 0.5: the Kurihara project. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2015;5(2):221–32. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1159/000380816.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Park DC, Lodi-Smith J, Drew L, Haber S, Hebrank A, Bischof GN, et al. The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: the Synapse Project. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(1):103–12. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0956797613499592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Peña J, Brody N. Increasing Cognitive Readiness Through Computer and Videogame-Based Training. Commun Res. 2014;43(4):571–92. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0093650214554614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Pitkala KH, Routasalo P, Kautiainen H, Sintonen H, Tilvis RS. Effects of Socially Stimulating Group Intervention on Lonely, Older People’s Cognition: A Randomized, Controlled Trial. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19(7):654–63. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181f7d8b0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Tanaka M, Ishii A, Yamano E, Ogikubo H, Okazaki M, Kamimura K, et al. Effect of a human-type communication robot on cognitive function in elderly women living alone. Medical Science Monitor. 2012;18(9):Cr550-7. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.12659/msm.883350.

  70. Tesky VA, Thiel C, Banzer W, Pantel J. Effects of a group program to increase cognitive performance through cognitively stimulating leisure activities in healthy older subjects: The AKTIVA study. GeroPsych: J Gerontopsychol Geriatr Psychiatr. 2011;24(2):83-92. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1024/1662-9647/a000035.

  71. Vidovich MR, Lautenschlager NT, Flicker L, Clare L, McCaul K, Almeida OP. The PACE study: a randomized clinical trial of cognitive activity strategy training for older people with mild cognitive impairment. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2015;23(4):360–72. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jagp.2014.04.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Zimmermann N, Netto TM, Amodeo MT, Ska B, Fonseca RP. Working memory training and poetry-based stimulation programs: are there differences in cognitive outcome in healthy older adults? NeuroRehabilitation. 2014;35(1):159–70. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3233/nre-141104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Davidson RJ, McEwen BS. Social influences on neuroplasticity: stress and interventions to promote well-being. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(5):689–95. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/nn.3093.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Kelly ME, Duff H, Kelly S, McHugh Power JE, Brennan S, Lawlor BA, et al. The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: a systematic review. Syst Rev. 2017;6(1):259. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13643-017-0632-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Scarmeas N, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology. 2003;25(5):625–33. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Small BJ, Dixon RA, McArdle JJ, Grimm KJ. Do changes in lifestyle engagement moderate cognitive decline in normal aging? Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology. 2012;26(2):144–55. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/a0026579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Baker J, Berzins K, Canvin K. Chapter 5, Results of the behaviour change technique synthesis: the presence and frequency of behaviour change techniques in interventions. Components of interventions to reduce restrictive practices with children and young people in institutional settings: the Contrast systematic mapping review. Health and Social Care Delivery Research, No. 10.8. Southampton (UK): National Institute for Health and Care Research; 2022.

  78. Crawshaw J, Meyer C, Antonopoulou V, Antony J, Grimshaw JM, Ivers N, et al. Identifying behaviour change techniques in 287 randomized controlled trials of audit and feedback interventions targeting practice change among healthcare professionals. Implement Sci. 2023;18(1):63. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13012-023-01318-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. De Vasconcelos S, Toskin I, Cooper B, Chollier M, Stephenson R, Blondeel K, et al. Behaviour change techniques in brief interventions to prevent HIV, STI and unintended pregnancies: A systematic review. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(9):e0204088. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0204088.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. McHugh S, Sinnott C, Racine E, Timmons S, Byrne M, Kearney PM. ‘Around the edges’: using behaviour change techniques to characterise a multilevel implementation strategy for a fall prevention programme. Implementation Science. 2018;13(1). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13012-018-0798-6.

  81. Samdal GB, Eide GE, Barth T, Williams G, Meland E. Effective behaviour change techniques for physical activity and healthy eating in overweight and obese adults; systematic review and meta-regression analyses. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):42. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12966-017-0494-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Carraça E, Encantado J, Battista F, Beaulieu K, Blundell J, Busetto L, et al. Effective behavior change techniques to promote physical activity in adults with overweight or obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Reviews. 2021;22 Suppl 4(Suppl 4):e13258. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/obr.13258.

  83. Buchan DS, Ollis S, Thomas NE, Baker JS. Physical activity behaviour: an overview of current and emergent theoretical practices. J Obesity. 2012;2012:546459. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1155/2012/546459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Ashton LM, Sharkey T, Whatnall MC, Williams RL, Bezzina A, Aguiar EJ, et al. Effectiveness of interventions and behaviour change techniques for improving dietary intake in young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of RCTs. Nutrients. 2019;11(4):825. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/nu11040825.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Bicchieri C. Tools for Change. Norms in the Wild: How to Diagnose, Measure, and Change Social Norms: Oxford University Press; 2017. p. 142-62.

  86. Shaw PA, Sumner AL, Halton CC, Bailey SC, Wolf MS, Andrews EN, et al. “You’re more engaged when you’re listening to somebody tell their story”: A qualitative exploration into the mechanisms of the podcast ‘menopause: unmuted’ for communicating health information. Patient Educ Couns. 2022;105(12):3494–500. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.pec.2022.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Laurita AC, Hazan C, Spreng RN. An attachment theoretical perspective for the neural representation of close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2019;14(3):237–51. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/scan/nsz010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Leary MR. Emotional responses to interpersonal rejection. Dialogues Clin Neurosc. 2015;17(4):435-41. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.31887/DCNS.2015.17.4/mleary.

  89. Lara E, Martín-María N, De la Torre-Luque A, Koyanagi A, Vancampfort D, Izquierdo A, et al. Does loneliness contribute to mild cognitive impairment and dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Ageing Res Rev. 2019;52:7–16. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.arr.2019.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Tabaei-Aghdaei Z, McColl-Kennedy JR, Coote LV. Goal Setting and Health-Related Outcomes in Chronic Diseases: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Literature From 2000 to 2020. Med Care Res Rev. 2022;80(2):145–64. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/10775587221113228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Jeong YH, Healy LC, McEwan D. The application of Goal Setting Theory to goal setting interventions in sport: a systematic review. Int Rev Sport Exer Psychol. 1-26. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/1750984X.2021.1901298.

  92. Enright G, Allman-Farinelli M, Redfern J. Effectiveness of Family-Based Behavior Change Interventions on Obesity-Related Behavior Change in Children: A Realist Synthesis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(11):4099. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/ijerph17114099.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Höpfner J, Keith N. Goal missed, self hit: goal-setting, goal-failure, and their affective, motivational, and behavioral consequences. Front Psychol. 2021;12:12:704790. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.704790.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Schunk DH, DiBenedetto MK. Motivation and social cognitive theory. Contemp Educ Psychol. 2020;60:101832. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Nutbeam D, Lloyd JE. Understanding and Responding to Health Literacy as a Social Determinant of Health. Annu Rev Public Health. 2021;42:159–73. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-090419-102529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Koch A, Yzerbyt V, Abele A, Ellemers N, Fiske ST. Social evaluation: Comparing models across interpersonal, intragroup, intergroup, several-group, and many-group contexts. Advances in experimental social psychology: Elsevier Academic Press; 2021. p. 1-68, https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/bs.aesp.2020.11.001.

  97. Jagroep W, Cramm JM, Denktaş S, Nieboer AP. Health behaviours and well-being among older adults with a Surinamese migration background in the Netherlands. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):2006. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12889-022-14414-z.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Ms Manyanlin Soumaro and Ms Jenny Ma Rose Pinpin who assisted with the search strategy during their research placement.

Funding

This body of work was not funded.

JS receives funding from Western Sydney University Research Theme Fellowship. GZS’s contribution was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Investigator Grant (APP1195709). SS receives funding from the Dementia Australia Research Foundation Fellowship. As a medical research institute, NICM receives research grants and donations from foundations, universities, government agencies, individuals and industry. Sponsors and donors provide untied funding for work to advance the vision and mission of the Institute. The project that is the subject of this article was not undertaken as part of a contractual relationship with any organisation other than the funding declared above. It should also be noted that NICM conducts clinical trials relevant to this topic area, for which further details can be provided on request. GZS is affiliated with NICM and has no conflicts to declare.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JS: Conceptualisation, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, supervision, validation, visualisation, writing – original draft preparation, writing – review and editing VC: Data curation, Visualization, Writing – Original Draft and Preparation SS: Validation, Writing – Review and Editing CH: Validation, Writing – Review and Editing GZS: Validation, Writing – Review and Editing FM: Validation, Writing – Review and Editing

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joyce Siette.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Siette, J., Chong, V., Samtani, S. et al. A meta-analysis of behaviour change techniques in social interventions targeting improved cognitive function in older adults. BMC Public Health 25, 1158 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12889-025-22229-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12889-025-22229-x

Keywords